Abstract
Background: Decision support to facilitate informed consent is increasingly important for complicated medical tests. Here, we test a theoretical model of factors influencing decisional conflict in a study examining the effects of a decision support aid that was designed to assist patients at high risk for hereditary nonpolyposis colorectal cancer (CRC) deciding whether to pursue the microsatellite instability (MSI) test.
Methods: Participants were 239 CRC patients at high familial risk for a genetic mutation who completed surveys before and after exposure to the intervention. Half of the sample was assigned to the CD-ROM aid and half received a brief description of the test. Structural equation modeling was employed to examine associations among the intervention, knowledge, pros and cons to having MSI testing, self-efficacy, preparedness, and decisional conflict.
Results: The goodness of fit for the model was acceptable [FIML, full information maximum likelihood, χ2 (df = 280) = 392.24; P = 0.00]. As expected, the paths to decisional conflict were significant for postintervention pros of MSI testing (t = −2.43; P < 0.05), cons of MSI testing (t = 2.78; P < 0.05), and preparedness (t = −7.27; P < 0.01). The intervention impacted decisional conflict by increasing knowledge about the MSI test and knowledge exerted its effects on decisional conflict by increasing preparedness to make a decision about the test and by increases in perceived benefits of having the test.
Conclusion: Increasing knowledge, preparedness, and perceived benefits of undergoing the MSI test facilitate informed decision making for this test.
Impact: Understanding mechanisms underlying health decisions is critical for improving decisional support. Individuals with Lynch syndrome have an elevated lifetime risk of CRC. Risk of Lynch syndrome may be assessed with a tumor-based screening test (MSI testing or immunohistochemical tissue staining). Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev; 20(2); 249–54. ©2011 AACR.
Introduction
Individuals with positive MSI or IHC (immunohistochemical) results have a high likelihood of a germline genetic mutation consistent with Lynch syndrome. The Bethesda criteria distinguish important elements of personal, family, and clinical history to guide providers considering offering Lynch screening (1).
Informed consent is routinely obtained before DNA-based cancer predisposition genetic testing. Informed consent should educate patients about the risks, benefits, and alternatives of tests and prepare them to make the decision about testing (2–4). Provision of sufficient information is important before microsatellite instability (MSI) testing. First, MSI testing may indicate the need for further genetic counseling and germline mutation analysis. Second, the outcome of Lynch testing may have implications for family members. Finally, genetic risk information can be difficult to understand and thus adequate preparation about family risk for cancers associated with Lynch syndrome is necessary. Unfortunately, research suggests that colorectal cancer (CRC) patients have little knowledge of the MSI test (5).
Decision support aids (DA) have been shown to facilitate decision making in a variety of clinical contexts including cancer-related genetic testing (6–8). Decisional conflict is a key construct from the Ottawa Decision Support Framework (9), and reducing decisional conflict is a primary goal for DAs (10). Most DAs target decisional conflict reduction by addressing contributors to uncertainty including providing information about benefits and risks for options. Nonetheless, little is known about the mechanisms whereby DAs affect decisional conflict. A greater understanding of mechanisms may have implications for development of more effective DAs.
The current study tests a theoretical model (Fig. 1) of factors contributing to decisional conflict about having the MSI test. The most consistent effects of DAs are upon improved knowledge about test purpose and test outcomes (6, 7,11, 12). Therefore, we proposed that the primary influence of the DA would be on knowledge about the MSI and IHC tests. We propose 4 mediating influences on the association between knowledge and conflict: perceived benefits of MSI testing, perceived barriers to MSI testing, preparedness for making the decision, and self-efficacy. Greater knowledge is predicted to lower decisional conflict by helping patients feel more informed about the test's benefits and barriers/risks (13, 14). In line with previous literature, improved knowledge would increase satisfaction with the level of decision-making preparation (15). A possible mediating influence of preparedness on the association between knowledge and conflict has not previously been evaluated. A final mediator is self-efficacy defined as the level of confidence in one's ability to understand the procedure and outcomes. DAs have been shown to increase self-efficacy (16). However, whether self-efficacy is a mechanism for the effects of improved knowledge on reduced decisional conflict has not been studied.
Proposed model of associations between knowledge, attitudes, and decisional conflict.
In the following analysis, we employed structural equation modeling to examine a mediational model of associations among knowledge, attitudes, and decisional conflict using baseline and follow-up data collected from a randomized trial of a CD-ROM intervention designed to facilitate informed consent for MSI testing (5,14).
Methods
Overview of the randomized study
Details of the methods are reported previously (14). Briefly, participants were 239 individuals with CRC meeting the revised Bethesda criteria (1) who were offered MSI testing. Participants were excluded if they met more stringent “Amsterdam criteria” (17). Informed consent was signed and the participant completed the baseline survey. Participants were then assigned to either education (E) only or E + CD-ROM. For E participants, the health educator provided a brief, standardized description of the MSI and IHC tests. For E + CD-ROM participants, education was followed by viewing the CD-ROM. Participants were permitted to review the CD-ROM at their own pace. Median usage time was 24 minutes (range 10–106 minutes). Two weeks later, the follow-up assessment was completed. Of the 319 patients approached, 239 consented, completed the baseline survey (75%) and 208 (75%) completed the follow-up survey. Approximately 90% of participants were white, 44% were college educated, and 69% were married.
Measures of MSI knowledge (17-item face valid true-false measure) and IHC knowledge (8-item face valid true-false measure) were developed by the investigative team. A 14-item Benefits of MSI testing measure and a 10-item Cons of MSI testing measure were adapted from a BRCA1/2 measure (18). A 4-item Self-Efficacy scale was modeled after the work of Bunn and O'Connor (19). Satisfaction with Preparation was measured with the 9-item Satisfaction with the Decision-making Process Questionnaire (20). A 10-item Completeness of Preparation scale was developed to examine how prepared participants felt to make the decision whether to have the MSI test. Decisional Conflict was assessed using the 16-item Decisional Conflict Scale (21).
Overview of the analytic strategy
Initially, all variables were assessed for normality. All were normally distributed with no outliers. A latent variable structural equation model (SEM) was used to test the theoretical framework (Fig. 1). Before conducting the SEM, individual items representing each latent construct were randomly combined into item parcels reflecting that particular construct. Two item parcels for each construct were developed. The 5 theoretically relevant subscales for decisional conflict were used as its manifest indicators.
The analyses employed LISREL v.8.8. Because of the small amount of missing data (8.85%), full information maximum likelihood (FIML) parameter estimation was used. In the missing data case, goodness of fit indicators (GFI) include χ2 and the point estimate of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). RMSEA is used in a GFI significance test. Good fit is indicated by a nonsignificant χ2 and a nonsignificant RMSEA which should be less than 0.08 (22).
Results
Table 1 contains descriptive statistics and alphas for model variables.
Descriptive statistics and factor loadings for variables used in structural equation analysis
Given the centrality of postintervention knowledge in the theoretical framework (Fig. 1), we examined knowledge variables predicted by treatment arm and baseline MSI and IHC knowledge. The GFIs indicated a good fit to the data [χ2 (df = 20) = 28.20; P = 0.10; RMSEA = 0.041 (90% CI: 0.0–0.074); P = 0.63)]. As reported previously (14), treatment arm was a significant predictor of postintervention MSI and IHC knowledge (t = 2.29; P < 0.05; t = 3.03; P < 0.05, respectively). However, contrary to expectations, post-IHC knowledge was not predicted by its baseline counterpart. Removal of this path continued to yield a good fit (baseline IHC was deleted from subsequent model assessments).
Because the GFIs indicated a reasonably good fit to the data, modification indices (MI) were examined to determine if conceptually supported model changes could be identified. The MIs suggested that preintervention MSI knowledge should predict postintervention IHC knowledge. Freeing this path continued to yield a good fit [χ2 (df = 9) = 10.92; P = 0.28; RMSEA = 0.030 (90% CI: 0.0–0.082); P = 0.68]. The path was positive and significant (t = 3.06; P < 0.05).
Next, we examined intervening variables hypothesized to be predictors of decisional conflict. The introduction of pre- and postintervention MSI pros yielded a well-fitting model [χ2 (df = 37) = 33.26; P = 0.64; RMSEA = 0.00 (90% CI: 0.0–0.039); P = 0.99]. However, postintervention IHC knowledge did not predict postintervention MSI pros. Constraining this path to zero continued to yield a good model. The path from postintervention MSI knowledge was positive and significant (t = 2.43; P < 0.05).
MSI cons were examined next. The model continued to fit well [χ2 (df = 77) = 86.23; P = 0.22; RMSEA = 0.02 (90% CI: 0.0–0.044); P = 0.99]. However, neither postintervention MSI nor postintervention IHC knowledge predicted postintervention MSI cons. Nonetheless, the model GFIs remained acceptable.
The addition of pre- and postdecisional self-efficacy demonstrated that postintervention MSI knowledge did not predict postintervention decisional self-efficacy. However, model fit was still quite acceptable [χ2 (df = 139) = 157.00; P = 0.14; RMSEA = 0.023 (90% CI: 0.0–0.040); P = 1.00]. Constraining this path to zero still yielded an acceptable fit. The relationship between post-IHC knowledge and postintervention self-efficacy was positive (t = 5.84; P < 0.01).
The introduction of preparedness into the model indicated that the path linking postintervention IHC knowledge to preparedness was nonsignificant. Its removal led to an acceptable [χ2 (df = 174) = 219.21; P = 0.011; RMSEA = 0.033 (90% CI: 0.017–0.046; P = 0.99] fit.
Although the model in Figure 1 represents the hypothesis that the path from treatment arm to preparedness is only indirect (i.e., working through the knowledge constructs), the MIs suggested a direct path as well. freeing the direct path demonstrated that it was significant (t = 2.74; P < 0.05). Greater preparedness was reported by those in the E + CD condition compared with those in the E condition. Model fit was still acceptable [χ2 (df = 173) = 212.24; P = 0.023; RMSEA = 0.031 (90% CI: 0.012–0.044); P = 0.99].
A test of the full model depicted in Figure 2 showed that model GFI was quite acceptable [χ2 (df = 280) = 392.24; P = 0.00; RMSEA = 0.041 (90% CI: 0.031–0.050); P = 0.99]. As expected, the paths to decisional conflict were significant for postintervention MSI pros (t = −2.43; P < 0.05), MSI cons (t = 2.78; P < 0.05), and preparedness (t = −7.27; P < 0.01). Less decisional conflict was reported by those reporting more MSI pros, better preparedness, and fewer MSI cons. However, although postintervention IHC knowledge predicted greater decisional self-efficacy, the latter did not predict decisional conflict. Our model had a lower (i.e., a better fit) AIC (23) than an alternate model in which decisional conflict predicted MSI pros, MSI cons, and preparedness.
Results of SEM of associations between knowledge, attitudes, and decisional conflict.
Discussion
The SEM explained 47% of the variance in decisional conflict suggesting that the intervention along with knowledge and decisional processes played a strong role in predicting conflict. The intervention increased MSI knowledge which, in turn, influenced preparedness and pros, and both knowledge- and preparedness-mediated associations with decisional conflict. In this context, obtaining knowledge helped patients feel more prepared to make the decision and to appreciate the possible benefits of the MSI test.
However, our hypothetical model was not completely supported by the data. First, although MSI cons contributed to decisional conflict, neither the intervention nor MSI and/or IHC knowledge reduced perceived cons. One possible explanation is that the content of the CD-ROM did not address some testing barriers such as the lack of desire to learn about familial risk and concerns about insurance discrimination. A second way the model was not supported was due to the associations among the intervention, IHC knowledge, and self-efficacy. The intervention increased IHC knowledge which then increased self-efficacy. However, neither IHC knowledge nor self-efficacy was associated with other decisional processes or decisional conflict. Two contributing factors may include that there was less informational content about IHC in the CD-ROM, and that the pros, cons, and preparedness items asked about MSI testing, making IHC knowledge a less critical component of the decision-making process evaluated here.
In terms of limitations, although our approach was strengthened by the incorporation of longitudinal data, any approach that searches for a best fitting model by using MIs, such as SEM, is susceptible to capitalization on chance (24). Replication with another sample would be necessary. Also, because decisional conflict and follow-up mediator variables were measured at the same time point, causality cannot be proven but only inferred by the superior AIC of the final model. Although we tested an alternative model which did not fit as well as the proposed model, it is still possible that reduced decisional conflict leads to increased perceptions of preparedness and greater pros. Important limitations to the study's sample also exist as it is not representative of the U.S. cancer population by race/ethnicity, socioeconomic level, and treatment at an academic medical center.
Our results suggest that DAs affect decisional conflict through multiple pathways including knowledge-dependent and knowledge-independent paths, attitude changes, and improved feelings of preparation. Further research to understand the importance of different pathways while varying decisional context (difficult vs. easy) and the decision maker (educated vs. uneducated) may better elucidate the decision process. Future DAs may also have a greater impact if content addressed more barriers. As noted by Farrell and colleagues (25), attitudes about testing should be taken into account when counseling patients about test benefits and risks. From a clinical perspective, these findings illuminate the important role of knowledge, preparedness, and perceived benefits of testing in reducing uncertainty in the decision making context.
Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest
No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed.
Grant Support
Funded by grant CA109332 from the National Cancer Institute and P30 CA006927 to Fox Chase Cancer Center, and by an Established Investigator in Cancer Prevention and Control Award to S.L. Manne by the National Cancer Institute (K05 CA109008).
The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked advertisement in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Acknowledgments
We thank Mary Daly, Jason Driesbaugh, Zhora Catts, Maryann Krayger, Deborah Lee, Cheri Manning, Kristen Shannon, Hetal Vig, Marianna Silverman, Stacy McConnell, and Sara Worhach. We also thank the physicians at Fox Chase Cancer Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, and Christiana Care Health System for referring patients to this study.
- Received June 28, 2010.
- Revision received December 13, 2010.
- Accepted December 15, 2010.
- ©2011 American Association for Cancer Research.