Skip to main content
  • AACR Publications
    • Blood Cancer Discovery
    • Cancer Discovery
    • Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention
    • Cancer Immunology Research
    • Cancer Prevention Research
    • Cancer Research
    • Clinical Cancer Research
    • Molecular Cancer Research
    • Molecular Cancer Therapeutics

AACR logo

  • Register
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Advertisement

Main menu

  • Home
  • About
    • The Journal
    • AACR Journals
    • Subscriptions
    • Permissions and Reprints
    • Reviewing
  • Articles
    • OnlineFirst
    • Current Issue
    • Past Issues
    • CEBP Focus Archive
    • Meeting Abstracts
    • Progress and Priorities
    • Collections
      • COVID-19 & Cancer Resource Center
      • Disparities Collection
      • Editors' Picks
      • "Best of" Collection
  • For Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Author Services
    • Best of: Author Profiles
    • Informing Public Health Policy
    • Submit
  • Alerts
    • Table of Contents
    • Editors' Picks
    • OnlineFirst
    • Citation
    • Author/Keyword
    • RSS Feeds
    • My Alert Summary & Preferences
  • News
    • Cancer Discovery News
  • COVID-19
  • Webinars
  • Search More

    Advanced Search

  • AACR Publications
    • Blood Cancer Discovery
    • Cancer Discovery
    • Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention
    • Cancer Immunology Research
    • Cancer Prevention Research
    • Cancer Research
    • Clinical Cancer Research
    • Molecular Cancer Research
    • Molecular Cancer Therapeutics

User menu

  • Register
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention
Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention
  • Home
  • About
    • The Journal
    • AACR Journals
    • Subscriptions
    • Permissions and Reprints
    • Reviewing
  • Articles
    • OnlineFirst
    • Current Issue
    • Past Issues
    • CEBP Focus Archive
    • Meeting Abstracts
    • Progress and Priorities
    • Collections
      • COVID-19 & Cancer Resource Center
      • Disparities Collection
      • Editors' Picks
      • "Best of" Collection
  • For Authors
    • Information for Authors
    • Author Services
    • Best of: Author Profiles
    • Informing Public Health Policy
    • Submit
  • Alerts
    • Table of Contents
    • Editors' Picks
    • OnlineFirst
    • Citation
    • Author/Keyword
    • RSS Feeds
    • My Alert Summary & Preferences
  • News
    • Cancer Discovery News
  • COVID-19
  • Webinars
  • Search More

    Advanced Search

Review

Uptake Rates for Breast Cancer Genetic Testing: A Systematic Review

Mary E. Ropka, Jennifer Wenzel, Elayne K. Phillips, Mir Siadaty and John T. Philbrick
Mary E. Ropka
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jennifer Wenzel
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Elayne K. Phillips
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mir Siadaty
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
John T. Philbrick
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
DOI: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-05-0002 Published May 2006
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Purpose: Individuals and families dealing with the possibility of hereditary cancer risk face numerous decisions, including whether to obtain genetic testing. The purpose of this article is to determine what is known about the rate at which people obtain cancer genetic testing.

Methods: Using MEDLINE, CINAHL, and PSYCHINFO plus reviewing reference lists of relevant articles, we identified 40 studies in May 2002 that addressed breast cancer–related decisions, enrolled adult participants, were published in 1990 or more recently, were peer-reviewed primary clinical studies, addressed genetic testing either alone or in combination with genetic counseling, and reported rates at which participants showed interest in and/or underwent cancer genetic testing. Information regarding study design, participants, and genetic testing uptake rates was recorded. Each article was reviewed for methodologic quality using a flexible quality review system applicable to all study types.

Results: Of the 40 studies, 25 provided information about hypothetical genetic testing decisions, 14 about real decisions, and 1 about both. Mean hypothetical uptake was 66% (range, 20-96%) and real uptake was 59% (range, 25-96%). Multivariate logistic regression analyses found that decision type (real/hypothetical), personal and family history of breast cancer, and variability in sampling strategy, recruitment setting, and criteria for real and hypothetical uptake were independently associated with uptake. Our systematic review identified additional explanations for uptake variability (investigator influences, small sample sizes, variability in target populations, lack of clearly described sampling strategies, sampling methods open to bias, and variability in reporting associated risk factors).

Conclusion: In addition to clinical characteristics, research methodologic issues are likely to be major determinants of variability in published breast cancer genetic testing uptake rates. An understanding of these issues will clarify to clinicians why their clinical experience may not be congruent with published rates and help guide future research. (Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2006;15(5):840–55)

  • hereditary breast cancer
  • genetic testing
  • systematic review
  • meta-analysis

Introduction

Individuals and families dealing with the possibility of hereditary cancer risk face diverse and multiple decisions, including whether to obtain cancer genetic testing. There are two broad classes of medical decisions: “effective-care” decisions and “preference-sensitive” decisions. The first class involves effective health services for which there are large proven benefits compared with the harms, such that there is an obvious “best choice.” The second involves preference-sensitive health services for which the best choice is not clear either because the benefit/harm ratios are uncertain or because they are sensitive to how a person values benefits versus harms. These two different classes of decisions have implications for appropriate strategies of risk communication and decision support (1-3). The decision about breast cancer genetic testing is a preference-sensitive decision. The goal of making a preference-sensitive decision is to make a “quality” decision rather than the “right” decision. A quality decision is defined as one that both is informed and results in a decision consistent with a person's values (4-6).

The decision whether to obtain breast cancer genetic testing can be complex for biologic, behavioral, and social reasons (7). Genetic testing for hereditary breast cancer currently involves two known mutations in tumor suppressor genes, BRCA1 and BRCA2. However, it is likely that other mutations exist that have not yet been identified. If a mutation is not found with current testing, the possibility exists that an unknown mutation is still present, resulting in lingering uncertainty. Even if a known mutation is found, penetrance is variable and unpredictable, again resulting in uncertainty. Thus, understanding the limitations of genetic testing can be complicated for patients and families making the decision whether to be tested (6, 8). Deciding who should be the first in a family to obtain genetic testing adds an additional level of complexity. It is desirable to start testing with a family member who has had breast cancer, but that person may not be interested in being tested. In addition, unlike many other health threats that affect only individuals, families are inherently and inevitably involved in an individual's decision to obtain cancer genetic testing and in dealing with hereditary breast cancer risk. This adds the challenge of effective communication and agreement among family members regarding the task of helping one individual to make a quality decision (9-13).

To better understand the breast cancer genetic testing situation, recent systematic reviews and meta-analyses have examined the psychological aspects of cancer genetic counseling (14-16) and cancer genetic testing (17); predicting individuals' risks of illness (18); measurement of psychological factors associated with genetic testing for hereditary breast, ovarian, and colon cancers (19); and social, ethical, and legal considerations associated with genetic cancer risk assessment technologies (20). However, reviews have not yet addressed the cancer genetic testing decision itself. Although many individual studies have addressed diverse aspects of cancer genetic testing, including factors that influence cancer genetic testing and the consequences of cancer genetic testing, no synthesis has been focused on the uptake decision to obtain cancer genetic testing (21).

The purpose of this systematic review is to determine what is known about the rate at which people decide to obtain breast cancer genetic testing. We classified studies that reported participants actually obtaining genetic testing as “real genetic testing decisions” and studies reporting subjects' “intent” to have genetic testing or “interest” in testing as “hypothetical genetic testing decisions.” Specific research questions include the following: (a) What uptake rates, real and hypothetical, have been reported; how much do they vary; and are hypothetical rates higher than real rates? (b) How are uptake rates, real and hypothetical, measured? (c) Are personal or family history of breast cancer, or other clinical factors, associated with uptake rates? (d) Are there issues of study methodology that influence or potentially bias uptake rates? To answer these questions, we identified, critically appraised, and synthesized evidence from studies about breast cancer genetic testing decisions.

Information from this systematic review may be helpful to individuals and families dealing with hereditary breast cancer risk, health care providers providing genetic services or referrals for cancer genetic services, health systems setting policy, and future research programs in the following ways: (a) assisting readers of the uptake literature to understand its strengths and weaknesses; (b) assisting in planning for genetic services and resources for different populations, regions, or situations, including estimating demand for limited genetic service resources and promoting access to genetic testing resources for the appropriate people; (c) improving understanding of factors that influence use of genetic testing services and resources; and (d) improving understanding of the problems and pitfalls of uptake-related research for those using uptake rates in research (6, 21-25).

Materials and Methods

Search Strategy and Data Sources

We sought all studies that (a) addressed breast cancer–related decisions, (b) enrolled adult (ages ≥18 years) participants, (c) were published in 1990 or more recently, (d) were peer-reviewed primary clinical studies, (e) addressed genetic testing either alone or in combination with genetic counseling, and (f) reported rates at which participants showed interest in and/or obtained cancer genetic testing. In May 2002, two separate searches were done addressing cancer genetic counseling and cancer genetic testing in each of three databases: MEDLINE, CINAHL, and PSYCHINFO. We did not include EMBASE or CANCERLIT because on a trial search they did not add to the completeness of our search results. Our search strategies were complex because, unlike some systematic review or meta-analysis topics, a single term could not be used to represent our search topic. For example, multiple terms had to be included in the search strategy, including “genetic testing,” “predictive testing,” “BRCA1/2 testing,” “gene$ test$.mp.,” “susceptibility test$.mp.,” “gene$ predict$ test$.mp.,” “gene$ susceptibility test$.mp.,” “personal risk$ predictive test$.mp.,” “cancer$ gene$ test$.mp.,” “brca1 test$.mp.,” and “((brca or brca2) and test$).mp.” In addition, developing the search strategies was complicated because the syntax of the first search, which was developed in MEDLINE, had to be adapted to accommodate syntax differences in CINAHL and PSYCHINFO. Finally, because we did not want to miss relevant articles, for our search strategies we purposefully emphasized higher sensitivity, or avoiding false negatives, over specificity, thus allowing more false positives. Inappropriate articles were eliminated in the subsequent article selection process. The search strategies for MEDLINE, CINAHL, and PSYCHINFO are available from the authors.

The three genetic counseling files, one from each database, were combined into one file (yield, 1,726 references) and the three genetic testing files were combined into a second file (yield, 1,449 references), and duplicates were removed from the two resulting files. The genetic counseling and genetic testing files were then combined (yield, 2,613 references) and duplicates were removed. Letters to editors, reviews, comments, and duplicates were removed, leaving one set of articles (yield, 1,887) to be screened for inclusion in this review.

Article Selection and Classification

Two reviewers independently evaluated each citation for possible inclusion, initially using the title and other information from the database citation. Disagreements were resolved by detailed review of the abstract and occasionally the full article. Of the 1,887 articles, 240 underwent this more detailed review. Reference lists of reviews on genetic counseling and testing, as well as articles included in our review, were examined for eligible studies not identified by the MEDLINE, CINAHL, and PSYCHINFO searches. Fourteen additional articles for possible inclusion were identified in this manner. As a final step to determine which articles to include, variables in each article were examined to find those that addressed a decision to obtain genetic testing. Forty studies met all selection criteria and constitute the basis for this review (8, 26-64).

When, in any given study, multiple populations were offered tests for different cancers, only data from the breast cancer group were used. Where both counseling and testing decisions were examined, only the testing data were included. We grouped results regarding study participants' “interest in” or “intent to have” genetic testing as “hypothetical genetic testing decisions.” We grouped studies reporting participants' rate of genetic testing as “real genetic testing decisions.” Real decisions were sometimes reported as “taking the test” or “drawing blood for testing.”

Data Abstraction

Information recorded for each study included type of decision (real or hypothetical), country where done, study design, participants' breast cancer personal history (present in all subjects or not), participants' breast cancer family history (present in all subjects or not), sampling approach, sample size, recruitment setting (community, hospital/clinic, both, or unknown), recruitment method, method of measuring hypothetical decisions of genetic testing interest or intent (exact question wording), operational definition of hypothetical uptake (how interest or intent was transformed into a dichotomous variable, yes/no), operational definition of real uptake (dichotomous variable, yes/no), genetic testing uptake rates for hypothetical and real decisions, the “window” of opportunity for subjects to have blood drawn for testing, interventions delivered, and groups for which uptake rates were reported. When a study reported more than one real uptake rate, we used the “blood drawn” or “test done” rate for statistical analysis. When a study evaluated uptake rates in more than one distinct group of subjects, we abstracted descriptive data and uptake rates for each group separately rather than for the overall study. For example, for a study that recruited two groups of women, one with a family history of breast cancer and a second without this family history (43), we considered the two groups to be different samples, each with its own descriptive data and uptake rate. In addition, when a study evaluated a variable for association with decisions regarding genetic testing uptake, details of the variable and statistical significance were recorded.

Quality Review of Articles

Each of the included articles was reviewed for methodologic quality using a flexible quality review system applicable to all study types. These six standards were adapted by the investigators from other reviews (65-69) and focused on methodologic issues important to study quality and minimizing bias pertinent to studies of uptake rates. The standards, their rationale, and study type for which each is relevant are as follows: standard 1: to allow understanding of the study subjects, the methods section provides clearly stated subject inclusion and exclusion criteria (applicable to all study types); standard 2: to allow an estimation of reproducibility and whether study results can be applied to other groups (external validity), the sampling strategy is described clearly enough so that it would be possible to assemble a similar group if the study were to be repeated (applicable to all study types); standard 3: to allow an understanding of the potential effect of incomplete participation, the number of individuals who refused to participate is reported (applicable to all study types); standard 4: to allow for assessment of the potential effect of study dropouts, the number of enrolled individuals who withdrew is reported and, if there were study dropouts, the number of dropouts is <10% (not applicable to correlational/descriptive studies); standard 5: to limit unplanned crossover of study groups, if there were different study groups, the groups are maintained throughout the study (not applicable to correlational/descriptive and qualitative studies); and standard 6: to ensure that compared groups are similar, except for factors that identify the different groups before an intervention (if there were more than one group in the study), simple demographic variables are similar between the groups at baseline, and if there are differences in these potential confounders, statistical adjustment for them is included in the analysis (not applicable to qualitative studies).

Two of the authors independently assessed the articles for study design and rated them for compliance with standards relevant to that design. Each article received a rating of 2 (complete compliance), 1 (partial compliance), 0 (noncompliance), or NA (not applicable) for each standard. The ratings of the two reviewers (J.T.P. and J.W.) were compared and discrepancies were resolved by discussion to achieve consensus. If agreement could not be achieved, a third reviewer (M.E.R.) evaluated the article.

Statistical Analysis

For each distinctive group of study subjects, we entered into a database the number of subjects, number of subjects choosing uptake, decision type (real or hypothetical), personal history of breast cancer (present in all subjects or not), family history of breast cancer (present in all subjects or not), recruitment setting (community, hospital/clinic, both, or unknown), sampling strategy (convenience or “reproducible”), real decision (immediate or delayed), and hypothetical decision (Likert/similar scale or dichotomous scale). Because for our analysis the dependent variable was response of each participant to the two-choice uptake question (yes/no), we concluded that the natural distributional assumption was the binomial distribution. Because we only had summary data on groups rather than data on individual subjects, we used the total number of subjects in each group and the number of “yes” respondents to define a grouped binary data structure. We then fitted logistic regression models to the data, where coefficients of regression were log odds ratios (OR). These coefficients were related to the dependent variable, uptake rate (r), by two transformations. The first was a transformation to an OR and the second was to a logarithm: log OR = log [r / (1 − r)]. When building the model, estimating the variables, and interpreting the results, we followed the methodologies explained in detail elsewhere (70, 71). This method allows for a more realistic distributional assumption for the dependent variable than the normal distribution, accounts for different sample sizes of the studies, has more statistical power, allows independent variables to be incorporated into the model, and both decreases heterogeneity and estimates effect of those variables on the dependent variable. We used SAS/STAT software version 9.1 to fit the models and estimate the variables (PROC GENMOD).

Results

Study Description

The 40 articles (8, 26-64) meeting the criteria to be included in this review are summarized in Table 1 (real decisions) and Table 2 (hypothetical decisions). There were many differences among the studies. Twenty-five studies provided information about hypothetical genetic testing decisions, 14 about real decisions, and 1 about both (36). Twenty-five employed a correlational/descriptive design, 6 were randomized controlled trials, 4 were qualitative, 2 were longitudinal studies (before/after, pre/post interventions), and 1 was a cohort study. They were done in six different countries, including 29 in the United States (8, 26-29, 34-37, 39, 40, 42-49, 51, 54, 55, 57, 58, 60-63, 72), 4 in Canada (41, 53, 56, 59), and 3 in the United Kingdom (33, 50, 64). Of the 40 studies, 7 used community settings (8, 29, 34, 42, 49, 57, 63), 27 clinic and/or hospital (26-28, 30-33, 35-37, 39, 43-48, 50, 52-55, 58, 60-62, 64), 5 both settings (40, 41, 51, 56, 59), and 1 not stated. Some studies targeted specific groups, including Ashkenazi Jewish women, known BRCA kindreds, and lesbian and bisexual women (28, 33, 42, 46, 48, 52, 53, 57, 72), whereas others recruited subjects from the general population (29, 34, 37, 41, 49). Twenty-three studies included only patients with a family history of breast cancer and 6 enrolled only patients with a personal history of breast cancer. Fifteen studies excluded patients with a personal history of breast cancer, whereas the number with personal history was unknown in 19 studies. No studies excluded patients with a family history of breast cancer, whereas the number with family history was unknown in 17 studies.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1.

Summary of reviewed studies according to real genetic testing decision

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2.

Summary of reviewed studies according to hypothetical genetic testing decision

Fourteen studies reported uptake rates of more than one distinct group of subjects, totaling 59 distinct groups among the 40 studies. One qualitative study reporting on hypothetical decisions did not report uptake quantitatively for its single group (34). Therefore, when reporting uptake rates, we use data from 58 groups (40 hypothetical, 18 real decisions) from the 40 studies. The mean number of patients per group was 178 (range, 4-761).

Research Question 1: What Uptake Rates, Real and Hypothetical, Have Been Reported; How Much Do They Vary; and Are Hypothetical Rates Higher than Real Rates?

The mean uptake rate of the 40 hypothetical decision groups was 66% and it was 59% for the 18 real decision groups. This difference is similar to that found by the one study reporting both hypothetical and real uptake rates in two patient groups (57% versus 51% and 61% versus 52%, respectively; ref. 36). There was a wide range of uptake rates for both the hypothetical decision groups (20-96%) and the real decision groups (25-96%). When decision type alone was entered into our multivariate logistic regression model evaluating associations with uptake rates, this difference favoring hypothetical decisions had a relatively small OR but was statistically significant [OR, 1.27; 95% confidence interval (95% CI), 1.16-1.39]. After the clinical and study methodology variables listed in Table 3 were added to the model, the relationship between hypothetical decision type and uptake rate was strengthened (OR, 2.03; 95% CI, 1.82-2.28).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3.

Multivariate logistic regression model for association with uptake

Research Question 2: How Are Uptake Rates, Real and Hypothetical, Measured?

Details of how real uptake rates were measured are presented in Table 1. To measure uptake, 13 of the 15 studies reporting real uptakes used either rates of drawing blood for genetic testing or performance of the genetic tests. Two studies (48, 54) recognized that sometimes subjects have genetic testing done but subsequently decline to receive the results. These studies reported two uptake rates, a “blood drawn” rate and a “test received” rate. However, in both studies, the blood drawn rate was only 2% higher than the test received rate. A second difference in how real uptake rates were determined had to do with the window of opportunity allowed for subjects to make an uptake decision. Five studies counted uptake decisions made at only one point in time, “immediate” testing after the initial genetic counseling session, not allowing “delayed” testing decisions made at a later time to be included (32, 36, 46, 54, 55). The other 10 real uptake studies counted subjects who could choose to delay testing for months or years after counseling.

Details of how hypothetical uptake rates were measured are presented in Table 2. Each of the 25 studies measured hypothetical uptake differently. Thirteen of the studies measured interest in genetic testing with a range of responses on four- or five-point Likert or similar scales, all different in wording. The other studies used different questions with dichotomous responses.

Research Question 3: Are Personal or Family History of Breast Cancer, or Other Clinical Factors, Associated with Uptake Rates?

The six studies that enrolled only patients with personal history of breast cancer had a mean uptake rate of 70% (range, 26-96%), whereas the remaining studies (15 that excluded patients with personal history, 19 personal history unknown) had a mean uptake of 63% (range, 24-93%). The 23 studies that enrolled only patients with family history of breast cancer had a mean uptake rate of 70% (range, 20-96%), whereas the remaining studies in which family history was unknown had a mean of 52% (range, 24-93%). Our multivariate logistic regression model that incorporated decision type, sampling strategy, and recruitment setting along with family and personal history (Table 3) found statistically significant associations of personal history (OR, 1.24; 95% CI, 1.11-1.39) and family history (OR, 2.11; 95% CI, 1.92-2.32) with uptake.

Many of the reviewed studies evaluated variables other than interventions for association with real and hypothetical uptake rates. Table 4 presents the results of the 16 studies that included personal characteristics, personal breast/ovarian cancer history, and/or family history of breast/ovarian cancer in this evaluation, organized according to variable. Eight used univariate statistical methods (26, 29, 31, 35, 39, 49, 54, 61), whereas the remainder employed multivariate analyses (37, 40, 42, 43, 47-49, 59). Variables found in individual studies to be associated with genetic testing uptake were older age (35, 47, 48), women of Ashkenazi Jewish heritage (47, 61), not married status (43), personal breast cancer history (47, 61), and family history of breast cancer (49, 59). However, these associations were inconsistent across studies, including personal and family history of breast cancer that in some studies failed to show a statistically significant correlation with uptake (39, 43, 47, 48, 54, 61) and in others showed an association in the opposite direction (31, 39, 59). As summarized in Table 4, there were many differences between studies, including varying variable definitions, widely disparate groups of subjects, and different statistical methods.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4.

Personal characteristics and cancer history variables evaluated for association with breast cancer genetic testing decision grouped according to variable and uptake type

Six studies evaluated the effect of educational and/or counseling interventions on hypothetical uptake rates (36, 48, 51, 57, 63, 64). Four (51, 57, 63, 64) reported reduced hypothetical uptake rates with these interventions and a fifth did not find a significant difference (36). The subjects, interventions, and measure of hypothetical uptake were different in each study. The absolute change in uptake rate ranged from a reduction of 33% to an increase of 8% (mean change, reduction of 10%). Two evaluated the effect of educational and/or counseling interventions on real uptake rates (36, 48) and did not find significant differences.

Research Question 4: Are There Issues of Study Methodology That Influence or Potentially Bias Uptake Rates?

Using our multivariate logistic regression model, we investigated whether recruitment setting, method of measuring uptake, and sampling strategy were associated with uptake. We found that studies recruiting patients in hospital/clinic settings and in combined hospital/clinic/community settings had significantly higher uptake rates than studies recruiting in community settings (Table 3) even when controlled for decision type and family and personal history.

To explore the degree to which uptake definition affects uptake rates, we created two additional regression models. The first evaluated in real studies whether uptake definitions that counted only immediate testing decisions differed from those that included delayed testing decisions. While controlling for personal and family history, sampling strategy, and recruitment setting, immediate testing decision studies had higher uptake rates (OR, 1.38; 95% CI, 1.14-1.66). The second model evaluated in the hypothetical studies whether uptake definitions using a Likert or similar scale differed from those using questions with dichotomous responses. While controlling for personal and family history, sampling strategy, and recruitment setting, dichotomous responses were associated with higher uptake rates (OR, 4.62; 95% CI, 4.07-5.24).

The last column in Tables 1 and 2 presents the quality ratings for the 40 studies. Standards 1 to 3 were concerned with assembly of study subjects. Whereas most studies provided clearly stated inclusion and exclusion criteria (standard 1, 31 studies rated partially or fully compliant) and reported the number of withdrawals (standard 3, 32 studies rated partially or fully compliant), less than half employed a sampling strategy that could reproducibly assemble study groups (standard 2). Whereas 4 enrolled consecutive series of eligible patients (50, 55, 61, 73), 2 enrolled the entire cohort of eligible subjects (33, 46), and 3 were random samples of a population (29, 49, 59), 30 studies assembled a “convenience” sample of patients. One study did not describe the sampling method (38). When sampling strategy was entered into our multivariate regression model (Table 3), convenience sampling was associated with higher uptake rates (OR, 1.75; 95% CI, 1.60-1.91) than the more reproducible sampling strategies.

Standards 4 to 6 were focused on assessing the potential for several important sources of bias. We rated them “not applicable” to more than half of the studies. There was general compliance with reporting and limiting study dropouts (standard 4, 7 of 9 applicable studies rated partially or fully compliant), maintaining patients in the groups to which they were originally assigned (standard 5, 8 of 8 partially or fully compliant), and limiting the potential for confounding variables (standard 6, 14 of 17 partially or fully compliant).

Discussion

Although some degree of variability is to be expected in the situation of a preference-based decision, we found the range of uptake rates, from 20% to 96%, to be surprisingly wide. Because it is easier to express a preference than to be tested, we expected hypothetical rates to be considerably higher than real rates. The difference that we found (66% versus 59%) was in the expected direction and statistically significant but only partly explained uptake rate variability. Because only one study measured real and hypothetical uptake in the same two groups (36), we were unable to estimate the extent to which real and hypothetical rates are correlated and the potential of hypothetical rates to be used in research and practice as a surrogate measure of real uptake rates.

A limitation of our review is that we last updated our literature search in May 2002. However, additional studies would not reduce the range of uptake rates or change the issues of research methodology described here.

Our review revealed that the definition of real uptake was not as clear and consistent as we anticipated. We expected the definition to be “blood drawn for testing” or “performance of genetic testing.” We found that sometimes study subjects consented to the blood draw but later decided not to receive the results of the genetic test, creating two possible ways to report real uptake: a “blood drawn” rate versus a “results received” rate. However, when these two rates were reported, they were quite similar (48, 54), suggesting that few people decline to receive test results once they have agreed to have their blood drawn. A second issue complicating measurement of real uptake rates was concerned with the time allowed after counseling for subjects to be counted as being tested. One third of the studies counted immediate testing decisions only, whereas the rest allowed patients who delayed decisions to be counted. Immediate decision rates were usually reported in studies in which data were collected prospectively, whereas delayed decision rates were often employed in retrospective studies. In our multivariate logistic model, the immediate/delayed decision variable was correlated with increased uptake, with immediate decision studies having higher rates. This seems contrary to what would be expected, because allowing delayed decisions to be included should and has been shown to increase uptake rates (74). However, a confounding factor may be that the immediate decision studies were more commonly done in research settings that produced higher uptake rates than the delayed decision studies, which were more often retrospective reports of routine clinical practice. More research is needed regarding how decisions to be tested are made and how they change over time.

Hypothetical uptake was defined differently in each of the studies. In about half of the studies, interest in genetic testing was rated using a Likert or similar scale, with uptake being defined by the authors from a range of responses on the scale. The other half used questions with dichotomous responses. Our multivariate logistic regression model showed that this was strongly correlated with hypothetical uptake. The potential for question wording to influence hypothetical uptake has been pointed out by others (22). Proof of this has been provided by a recent phone survey (75), which evaluated hypothetical uptake using three different instruments in the same cohort. Using different definitions of uptake, multiple rates were reported, ranging from 19% to 90%. Hypothetical uptake measures should be used as a surrogate for real uptake only in those situations where the two have been shown to be highly correlated. Further work is required to determine the best measures of hypothetical uptake and to evaluate them in a wide variety of subjects and clinical settings. In addition, more research is needed to determine the extent to which hypothetical uptake measures correlate with each other and with real uptake.

We found that personal and family history are important independent predictors of uptake. Our statistical analysis combining the results of the reviewed studies supports this conclusion. In addition, individual studies frequently found significant associations of uptake with personal and family history of breast cancer. As noted in Table 4, individual studies evaluated a wide range of variables for association with uptake. However, these associations often were inconsistent, possibly caused by such factors as differences in subject selection and variable definition, as well as statistical issues, including power. Educational and counseling interventions had a mixed effect on uptake, befitting a preference sensitive decision, either reducing uptake rates or having no effect.

In addition to the above, we have identified the following methodologic issues that could have an effect on or potentially bias uptake rate.

Investigator Influences. The presence of investigator influences on subjects can affect uptake rates. Some studies were observational studies of routine clinical care with minimal contact by the investigators and therefore little potential for influence on the subjects. Others were intervention studies with counseling or other special programs specifically designed to influence uptake rates. These interventions had varied effects. A particularly effective investigator inducement to obtain genetic testing was providing testing at no cost (61).

Small Sample Sizes. The 59 distinct subject groups ranged in size from 4 to 761. The mean size was 178 and the mean uptake rate was 63.5%. The 95% CI for this proportion is 56% to 71%. Thus, variability in sample size, and particularly small sample size, affects precision of the estimates. Variation in sample size is also an explanation for inconsistency in reporting of correlates with uptake rates. For example, a study (48) with 172 subjects did not find that personal cancer history significantly correlated with real uptake rates, whereas two larger studies (47, 61) with greater “statistical power” did.

Variability in Study Settings and Subjects. As shown in Tables 1 and 2, there was great variability in study settings and subject samples. Study subjects recruited at different points in the continuum of community/clinic/hospital are likely to have varying interest in genetic testing. A woman identified by random digit dialing would reasonably be expected to have different interest in genetic testing than a woman with a daughter who is newly diagnosed with breast cancer. These different settings and target populations would predictably contribute to variability in uptake rates. Our multivariate logistic regression model found recruitment setting to have the strongest association with uptake of the variables entered into model. Selection of target populations could also affect whether variables are associated with uptake rates. For example, in none of the five studies (47, 48, 54, 61, 62) evaluating associations with real uptake was family history found to be significant. The explanation for this could be that the subjects in all five studies were already at such high risk for breast cancer that family history was not significant as an additional predictor of uptake.

Lack of Clearly Described Sampling Strategy. Reproducible uptake rates depend on study methodologies that enable researchers to assemble populations that are similar in factors related to testing interest. Our first three quality review standards dealt with important issues related to selecting study subjects. We rated the studies being generally compliant with regard to specifying clearly stated inclusion and exclusion criteria (standard 1) and reporting refusals (standard 3). However, as indicated in Tables 1 and 2, we concluded that less than half of the 40 studies provided a clear enough description of sampling strategy to ensure assembly of a comparable group if the study were to be repeated. Only 15 of the 40 studies were rated as fully compliant with all three of these standards related to selecting study subjects. This lack of a reproducible sampling strategy among the reviewed studies undoubtedly contributes to variability in uptake estimates.

Sampling Methods Open to Bias. Thirty of the studies enrolled “convenience” samples of patients (i.e., patients identified in nonsystematic ways, such as volunteers in clinical settings or respondents to advertisements). Convenience sampling was associated with higher uptake rates than the more reproducible strategies (Table 3), perhaps reflecting volunteer bias. Studies based on convenience samples are less likely to produce the same results if replicated and also have limited generalizability. Only nine studies used the more rigorous, reproducible sampling methods of consecutive series, samples of the entire eligible cohort, or random samples of a population.

Although it is easier to do research with convenience samples and volunteers, the additional effort to assemble groups through such methods as random sampling and consecutive series is needed. In situations where this cannot be done, efforts to understand the potential for bias should be taken (e.g., by describing those who decline to volunteer as well the volunteers). Research focused on the differences between these groups could lead to more effective ways to approach those who decline.

Variability in Reporting Associated Risk Factors. We expected that uptake rates would be strongly related to certain risk factors, including family and personal history of breast cancer. We attempted to determine family and personal history of breast cancer in each of the 59 distinct patient groups. In 42 groups, family history status was clear; in 13, it was mixed (some with family history, some without); and in 4, it was unknown. In 39 groups, personal history status was clear; in 14, it was mixed (some with, some without); and in 6, it was unknown. Both family and personal history were specified in only 29 of the 59 groups. Lack of this information and other information related to testing prevented us from fully adjusting or stratifying uptake results for these potential confounding factors or effect modifiers.

Summary

Our review reveals the complexity of genetic testing uptake research. We believe that study methodology issues are major determinants of variability in published uptake rates, as important or more important than clinical characteristics, such as personal or family history of cancer. Greater understanding of these issues will clarify to clinicians why there is such great variability in the published literature and why their clinical experience may not be congruent with what they read. To interpret study results for application to practice, clinicians must pay particular attention to whether the method of studying uptake matches the way patients make decisions in their own setting. Before applying results to their patients, clinicians must determine that study samples are similar enough to their patients so that study results might reasonably be expected to be applicable to them. Because estimates of genetic testing uptake are so variable, interventions intended to affect uptake rates should be evaluated using randomized trials with attention to the methodologic issues identified in this review. Uptake rate is usually one of the primary outcome measures of genetic testing intervention studies, yet there is not a “correct” or “optimal” uptake rate because the decision is preference sensitive. Intervention studies should combine uptake rates with other outcomes, including measures of psychological effect (14, 15, 17), individual satisfaction with the decision, and consistency between an individual's values and their choice.

Footnotes

  • Grant support: National Cancer Institute, NIH grant K07 CA088989 (M.E. Ropka).

  • The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked advertisement in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

    • Accepted February 16, 2006.
    • Received January 4, 2005.
    • Revision received January 7, 2006.

References

  1. ↵
    O'Connor AM, Legare F, Stacey D. Risk communication in practice: the contribution of decision aids. BMJ 2003;327:736–40.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  2. O'Connor AM, Llewellyn-Thomas HA, Flood AB. Modifying unwarranted variations in health care: shared decision making using patient decision aids. Health Aff (Millwood) 2004;Suppl Web Exclusive: VAR 63–72.
  3. ↵
    Wennberg J. Unwarranted variations in healthcare delivery: implications for academic medical centres. BMJ 2002;325:961–4.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  4. ↵
    O'Connor AM, Mulley AG, Wennberg JE. Standard consultations are not enough to ensure decision quality regarding preference-sensitive options. J Natl Cancer Inst 2003;95:570–1.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  5. Briss P, Rimer B, Reilley B, et al. Promoting informed decisions about cancer screening in communities and healthcare systems. Am J Prev Med 2004;26:67–80.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Wang C, Gonzalez R, Marajver SD. Assessment of genetic testing and related counseling services: current research and future directions. Soc Sci Med 2004;58:1427–42.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  7. ↵
    Calzone KA, Biesecker BB. Genetic testing for cancer predisposition. Cancer Nurs 2002;25:15–25; quiz 6–7.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Press NA, Yasui Y, Reynolds S, Durfy SJ, Burke W. Women's interest in genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility may be based on unrealistic expectations. Am J Med Genet 2001;99:99–110.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. ↵
    Forrest K, Simpson SA, Wilson BJ, et al. To tell or not to tell: barriers and facilitators in family communication about genetic risk. Clin Genet 2003;64:317–26.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. Bowen DJ, Bourcier E, Press N, Lewis FM, Burke W. Effects of individual and family functioning on interest in genetic testing. Community Genet 2004;7:25–32.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. McGivern B, Everett J, Yager GG, Baumiller RC, Hafertepen A, Saal HM. Family communication about positive BRCA1 and BRCA2 genetic test results. Genet Med 2004;6:503–9.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  12. Van Riper M, McKinnon WC. Genetic testing for breast and ovarian cancer susceptibility: a family experience. J Midwifery Womens Health 2004;49:210–9.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  13. ↵
    McInerney-Leo A, Biesecker BB, Hadley DW, et al. BRCA1/2 testing in hereditary breast and ovarian cancer families II: impact on relationships. Am J Med Genet A 2005;133:165–9.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    Braithwaite D, Emery J, Walter F, Prevost AT, Sutton S. Psychological impact of genetic counseling for familial cancer: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Natl Cancer Inst 2004;96:122–33.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  15. ↵
    Broadstock M, Michie S, Marteau T. Psychological consequences of predictive genetic testing: a systematic review. Eur J Hum Genet 2000;8:731–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. ↵
    Meiser B, Halliday JL. What is the impact of genetic counselling in women at increased risk of developing hereditary breast cancer? A meta-analytic review. Soc Sci Med 2002;54:1463–70.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  17. ↵
    Butow PN, Lobb EA, Meiser B, Barratt A, Tucker KM. Psychological outcomes and risk perception after genetic testing and counselling in breast cancer: a systematic review. [Review] [26 refs]. Med J Aust 2003;178:77–81.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  18. ↵
    Shaw C, Abrams K, Marteau TM. Psychological impact of predicting individuals' risks of illness: a systematic review. Soc Sci Med 1999;49:1571–98.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. ↵
    Vadaparampil ST, Ropka M, Stefanek ME. Measurement of psychological factors associated with genetic testing for hereditary breast, ovarian, and colon cancers. Fam Cancer 2005;4:195–206.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. ↵
    Lee RC, Kmet L, Cook LS, Lorenzetti D, Godlovitch G, Einsiedel E. Risk assessment for inherited susceptibility to cancer: a review of the psychosocial and ethical dimensions. Genet Test 2005;9:66–79.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Miller SM, Bowen DJ, Campbell MK, et al. Current research promises and challenges in behavioral oncology: report from the American Society of Preventive Oncology annual meeting, 2002. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2004;13:171–80.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  22. ↵
    Bowen D, Patenaude AF, Vernon SW. Psychosocial issues in cancer genetics: from the laboratory to the public. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 1999;8:326–8.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  23. Feetham SL, Thomson EJ, Hinshaw AS. Nursing leadership in genomics for health and society. J Nurs Scholarsh 2005;37:102–10.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. Collins FS, Green ED, Guttmacher AE, Guyer MS. A vision for the future of genomics research. Nature 2003;422:835–47.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. ↵
    Gwyn K, Vernon SW, Conoley PM. Intention to pursue genetic testing for breast cancer among women due for screening mammography. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2003;12:96–102.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    Lerman C, Daly M, Masny A, Balshem A. Attitudes about genetic testing for breast-ovarian cancer susceptibility. J Clin Oncol 1994;12:843–50.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  27. ↵
    Lerman C, Seay J, Balshem A, Audrain J. Interest in genetic testing among first-degree relatives of breast cancer patients. Am J Med Genet 1995;57:385–92.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Struewing JP, Lerman C, Kase RG, Giambarresi TR, Tucker MA. Anticipated uptake and impact of genetic testing in hereditary breast and ovarian cancer families. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 1995;4:169–73.
    OpenUrlAbstract
  29. ↵
    Andrykowski MA, Munn RK, Studts JL. Interest in learning of personal genetic risk for cancer: a general population study. Prev Med 1996;25:527–36.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. ↵
    De Wit ACD, Tibben A, Frets PG, Meijers-Heijboer EJ, Devilee P, Niermeijer MF. Males at-risk for the BRCA1-gene, the psychological impact. Psychooncology 1996;5:251–7.
    OpenUrl
  31. ↵
    Julian-Reynier C, Eisinger F, Vennin P, et al. Attitudes towards cancer predictive testing and transmission of information to the family. J Med Genet 1996;33:731–6.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  32. ↵
    Patenaude AF, Schneider KA, Kieffer SA, et al. Acceptance of invitations for p53 and BRCA1 predisposition testing: factors influencing potential utilization of cancer genetic testing. Psychooncology 1996;5:241–50.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  33. ↵
    Watson M, Lloyd SM, Eeles R, et al. Psychosocial impact of testing (by linkage) for the BRCA1 breast cancer gene: an investigation of two families in the research setting. Psychooncology 1996;5:233–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  34. ↵
    Bernhardt BA, Geller G, Strauss M, et al. Toward a model informed consent process for BRCA1 testing: a qualitative assessment for women's attitudes. J Genet Couns 1997;6:207–22.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Jacobsen PB, Valdimarsdottir HB, Brown KL, Offit K. Decision-making about genetic testing among women at familial risk for breast cancer. Psychosom Med 1997;59:459–66.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Lerman C, Biesecker B, Benkendorf JL, et al. Controlled trial of pretest education approaches to enhance informed decision-making for BRCA1 gene testing. J Natl Cancer Inst 1997;89:148–57.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  37. ↵
    Tambor ES, Rimer BK, Strigo TS. Genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility: awareness and interest among women in the general population. Am J Med Genet 1997;68:43–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  38. ↵
    Braczkowski R, Nowakowska-Zajdel E, Zubelewicz B, Muc-Wierzgon M, Romanowski W. Genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility: Polish women's attitudes. J Exp Clin Cancer Res 1998;17:299–302.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  39. ↵
    Mogilner A, Otten M, Cunningham JD, Brower ST. Awareness and attitudes concerning BRCA gene testing. Ann Surg Oncol 1998;5:607–12.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Bluman LG, Rimer BK, Berry DA, et al. Attitudes, knowledge, and risk perceptions of women with breast and/or ovarian cancer considering testing for BRCA1 and BRCA2. J Clin Oncol 1999;17:1040–6.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  41. ↵
    Cappelli M, Surh L, Humphreys L, et al. Psychological and social determinants of women's decisions to undergo genetic counseling and testing for breast cancer. Clin Genet 1999;55:419–30.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  42. ↵
    Durfy SJ, Bowen DJ, McTiernan A, Sporieder J, Burke W. Attitudes and interest in genetic testing for breast and ovarian cancer susceptibility in diverse groups of women in western Washington. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 1999;8:369–75.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    Lipkus IM, Iden D, Terrenoire J, Feaganes JR. Relationships among breast cancer concern, risk perceptions, and interest in genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility among African-American women with and without a family history of breast cancer. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 1999;8:533–9.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  44. ↵
    Ludman EJ, Curry SJ, Hoffman E, Taplin S. Women's knowledge and attitudes about genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility. Eff Clin Pract 1999;2:158–62.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  45. ↵
    Ryan EL, Skinner CS. Risk beliefs and interest in counseling: focus-group interviews among first-degree relatives of breast cancer patients. J Cancer Educ 1999;14:99–103.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  46. ↵
    Smith KR, West JA, Croyle RT, Botkin JR. Familial context of genetic testing for cancer susceptibility: moderating effect of sibling's test results on psychological distress one to two weeks after BRCA1 mutation testing. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 1999;8:385–92.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  47. ↵
    Armstrong K, Calzone K, Stopfer J, Fitzgerald G, Coyne J, Weber B. Factors associated with decisions about clinical BRCA1/2 testing. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2000;9:1251–4.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  48. ↵
    Biesecker BB, Ishibe N, Hadley DW, et al. Psychosocial factors predicting BRCA1/BRCA2 testing decisions in members of hereditary breast and ovarian cancer families. Am J Med Genet 2000;93:257–63.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  49. ↵
    Bosompra K, Flynn BS, Ashikaga T, Rairikar CJ, Worden JK, Solomon LJ. Likelihood of undergoing genetic testing for cancer risk: a population-based study. Prev Med 2000;30:155–66.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  50. ↵
    Broadstock M, Michie S, Gray J, Mackay J, Marteau TM. The psychological consequences of offering mutation searching in the family for those at risk of hereditary breast and ovarian cancer-a pilot study. Psychooncology 2000;9:537–48.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  51. ↵
    Burke W, Culver JO, Bowen D, et al. Genetic counseling for women with an intermediate family history of breast cancer. Am J Med Genet 2000;90:361–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    Julian-Reynier C, Sobol H, Sevilla C, Nogues C, Bourret P. Uptake of hereditary breast/ovarian cancer genetic testing in a French national sample of BRCA1 families. The French Cancer Genetic Network. Psychooncology 2000;9:504–10.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  53. ↵
    Phillips KA, Warner E, Meschino WS, et al. Perceptions of Ashkenazi Jewish breast cancer patients on genetic testing for mutations in BRCA1 and BRCA2. Clin Genet 2000;57:376–83.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    Schwartz MD, Hughes C, Roth J, et al. Spiritual faith and genetic testing decisions among high-risk breast cancer probands. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2000;9:381–5.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    Wood ME, Mullineaux L, Rahm AK, Fairclough D, Wenzel L. Impact of BRCA1 testing on women with cancer: a pilot study. Genet Test 2000;4:265–72.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  56. ↵
    Cappelli M, Surh L, Walker M, et al. Psychological and social predictors of decisions about genetic testing for breast cancer in high-risk women. Psychol Health Med 2001;6:321–33.
    OpenUrl
  57. ↵
    Schwartz MD, Benkendorf J, Lerman C, Isaacs C, Ryan-Robertson A, Johnson L. Impact of educational print materials on knowledge, attitudes, and interest in BRCA1/BRCA2. Cancer 2001;92:932–40.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  58. ↵
    Velicer CM, Taplin S. Genetic testing for breast cancer: where are health care providers in the decision process? Genet Med 2001;3:112–9.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  59. ↵
    Bottorff JL, Ratner PA, Balneaves LG, et al. Women's interest in genetic testing for breast cancer risk: the influence of sociodemographics and knowledge. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2002;11:89–95.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  60. ↵
    Clark S, Bluman LG, Borstelmann N, et al. Patient motivation, satisfaction, and coping in genetic counseling and testing for BRCA1 and BRCA2. J Genet Couns 2000;9:219–35.
    OpenUrl
  61. ↵
    Lee SC, Bernhardt BA, Helzlsouer KJ. Utilization of BRCA1/2 genetic testing in the clinical setting: report from a single institution. Cancer 2002;94:1876–85.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  62. ↵
    Peterson EA, Milliron KJ, Lewis KE, Goold SD, Merajver SD. Health insurance and discrimination concerns and BRCA1/2 testing in a clinic population. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 2002;11:79–87.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  63. ↵
    Green MJ, Biesecker BB, McInerney AM, Mauger D, Fost N. An interactive computer program can effectively educate patients about genetic testing for breast cancer susceptibility. Am J Med Genet 2001;103:16–23.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  64. ↵
    Brain K, Norman P, Gray J, Rogers C, Mansel R, Harper P. A randomized trial of specialist genetic assessment: psychological impact on women at different levels of familial breast cancer risk. Br J Cancer 2002;86:233–8.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  65. ↵
    Liddle J, Williamson M, Irwig L. Method for evaluating research guideline evidence. Australia: NSW Department of Health; 1996. p. 1–29.
  66. Moher D, Cook DJ, Eastwood S, Olkin I, Rennie D, Stroup DF. Improving the quality of reports of meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials: the QUOROM statement. QUOROM Group. Br J Surg 2000;87:1448–54.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  67. West S, King V, Carey TS, et al. Systems to rate the strength of scientific evidence. AHRQ Publication No. 02-E016. Rockville, MD: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality. April 2002.
  68. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, et al. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA 2000;283:2008–12.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  69. ↵
    NHS Centre for Reviews and Dissemination. Undertaking systematic reviews of research on effectiveness. CRD Report No. 4, 2nd ed. York (United Kingdom): University of York; 2001.
  70. ↵
    Siadaty MS, Shu J. Proportional odds ratio model for comparison of diagnostic tests in meta-analysis. BMC Med Res Methodol 2004;4:27.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  71. ↵
    Siadaty MS, Philbrick J, Heim SW, Schectman JM. Repeated-measures modeling improved comparison of diagnostic tests in meta-analysis of dependent studies. J Clin Epidemiol 2004;57:698–710.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  72. ↵
    Patenaude AF. The genetic testing of children for cancer susceptibility: ethical, legal, and social issues. Behav Sci Law 1996;14:393–410.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  73. ↵
    Brain K, Gray J, Norman P, et al. Randomized trial of a specialist genetic assessment service for familial breast cancer. J Natl Cancer Inst 2000;92:1345–51.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  74. ↵
    Brooks L, Lennard F, Shenton A, et al. BRCA 1/2 predictive testing: a study of uptake in two centres. Eur J Hum Genet 2004;12:654–62.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  75. ↵
    Bottorff JL, Ratner PA, Richardson C, et al. The influence of question wording on assessments of interest in genetic testing for breast cancer risk. Psychooncology 2003;12:720–8.
    OpenUrlPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top
Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers & Prevention: 15 (5)
May 2006
Volume 15, Issue 5
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)

Sign up for alerts

View this article with LENS

Open full page PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for sharing this Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Uptake Rates for Breast Cancer Genetic Testing: A Systematic Review
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Uptake Rates for Breast Cancer Genetic Testing: A Systematic Review
Mary E. Ropka, Jennifer Wenzel, Elayne K. Phillips, Mir Siadaty and John T. Philbrick
Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev May 1 2006 (15) (5) 840-855; DOI: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-05-0002

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Uptake Rates for Breast Cancer Genetic Testing: A Systematic Review
Mary E. Ropka, Jennifer Wenzel, Elayne K. Phillips, Mir Siadaty and John T. Philbrick
Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev May 1 2006 (15) (5) 840-855; DOI: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-05-0002
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Introduction
    • Materials and Methods
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Summary
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Advertisement

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Epigenetic alterations and cervical cancer development
  • Area-Level Variation and HPV Vaccination
  • Lessons Learned from Setting Up a Prospective Study
Show more Review
  • Home
  • Alerts
  • Feedback
  • Privacy Policy
Facebook   Twitter   LinkedIn   YouTube   RSS

Articles

  • Online First
  • Current Issue
  • Past Issues

Info for

  • Authors
  • Subscribers
  • Advertisers
  • Librarians

About Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention

  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Permissions
  • Submit a Manuscript
AACR logo

Copyright © 2021 by the American Association for Cancer Research.

Cancer Epidemiology, Biomarkers & Prevention
eISSN: 1538-7755
ISSN: 1055-9965

Advertisement